Université de Fribourg

Spatial-numerical associations in 1st-graders: Evidence from a manual-pointing task

Möhring, Wenke ; Ishihara, Masami ; Curiger, Jacqueline ; Frick, Andrea

In: Psychological Research, 2019, vol. 83, p. 855-893

The current study investigated whether children’s mental representations of numbers are organized spatially at the onset of formal schooling using a manual-pointing task. First- graders (N = 77) saw four numbers (1, 3, 7, 9) presented randomly in four spatial positions (extreme left, left, right, extreme right) on a touch screen. In a Go/No-Go task, children were asked to press the appearing...

Université de Fribourg

Touching up mental rotation : Effects of manual experience on 6-month-old infants’ mental object rotation

Möhring, Wenke ; Frick, Andrea

In: Child Development, 2013, vol. 84, no. 5, p. 1554-1565

In this study, 6-month-olds’ ability to mentally rotate objects was investigated using the violation-of-expectation paradigm. Forty infants watched an asymmetric object being moved straight down behind an occluder. When the occluder was lowered, it revealed the original object (possible) or its mirror image (impossible) in one of five orientations. Whereas half of the infants were allowed...

Université de Fribourg

Mental object rotation and motor development in 8- and 10-month-old infants

Frick, Andrea ; Möhring, Wenke

In: Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2013, vol. 115, no. 4, p. 708-720

Recent evidence indicates that 6-month-old infants’ mental rotation of objects profits from prior manual experience, whereas observational experience does not have the same beneficial effect (Möhring, W. & Frick, A., 2013, Child Development). The present study investigated whether older infants, at 8 and 10 months of age, succeed in this task after observational experience only, and...

Université de Fribourg

Using a touch screen paradigm to assess the development of mental rotation between 3½ and 5½ years of age

Frick, Andrea ; Ferrara, Katrina ; Newcombe, Nora S.

In: Cognitive Processing, 2013, vol. 14, no. 2, p. 117-127

Mental rotation is an important spatial skill. However, there is controversy concerning its early development and susceptibility to intervention. In the present study, we assessed individual differences in the mental rotation abilities of children between 3½ and 5½ years of age, using a touch screen paradigm to simplify task demands. A figure or its mirror image was presented in 8 different...

Université de Fribourg

Development of mental rotation in 3- to 5-year-old children

Frick, Andrea ; Hansen, Melissa A. ; Newcombe, Nora S.

In: Cognitive Development, 2013, vol. 28, no. 4, p. 386-399

We assessed 3- to 5-year-olds’ mental rotation abilities using a new puzzle paradigm. It allows for assessment of mental rotation abilities in children younger than 5 years, using a task comparable to ones used with older children and adults. Children saw pairs of asymmetrical ghost figures, either as three-dimensional cut-outs or two- dimensional paper versions, in seven orientations. One...

Université de Fribourg

Zooming in on spatial scaling: Preschool children and adults use mental transformations to scale spaces

Möhring, Wenke ; Newcombe, Nora S. ; Frick, Andrea

In: Developmental Psychology, 2014, vol. 50, no. 5, p. 1614-1619

Spatial scaling is an important prerequisite for many spatial tasks and involves an understanding of how distances in different-sized spaces correspond. Previous studies have found evidence for such an understanding in preschoolers; however, the mental processes involved remain unclear. The present study investigated whether children and adults use mental transformations to scale distances in...

Université de Fribourg

Young children’s perception of diagrammatic representations

Frick, Andrea ; Newcombe, Nora S.

In: Spatial Cognition & Computation, 2015, vol. 15, no. 4, p. 227-245

Diagrams and pictorial representations are common in children's lives and require abstraction away from visual perception. In three experiments, we investigated 4- to 8- year-olds’ comprehension of such representations. In Experiment 1 (N = 80), children were shown photographs of geometric objects and asked to choose the corresponding line drawing from among sets of four, or vice versa....

Université de Fribourg

Using mental transformation strategies for spatial scaling: Evidence from a discrimination task

Möhring, Wenke ; Newcombe, Nora S. ; Frick, Andrea

In: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2016, vol. 42, no. 9, p. 1473-1479

Spatial scaling, or an understanding of how distances in different-sized spaces relate to each other, is fundamental for many spatial tasks and relevant for success in numerous professions. Previous research has suggested that adults use mental transformation strategies to mentally scale spatial input, as indicated by linear increases in response times and accuracies with larger scaling...

Université de Fribourg

The relation between spatial perspective taking and inhibitory control in 6-year-old children

Frick, Andrea ; Baumeler, Denise

In: Psychological Research, 2017, vol. 81, no. 4, p. 730-739

Developmental research on spatial perspective taking has shown that young children are able to solve perspective-taking problems under favorable circumstances, but they have difficulties succeeding in classic tasks involving a conflict between one’s own perspective and that of another observer. To date, little is known about the reasons for young children’s difficulties in dealing with...